Skip to main content

From Distraction to Mindfulness: Psychological and Neural Mechanisms of Attention Strategies in Self-Regulation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Handbook of Biobehavioral Approaches to Self-Regulation

Abstract

The current chapter examines attention strategies that may facilitate self-regulation. In particular, we focus on the attention strategies of distraction and mindfulness. By distraction, we mean shifting attention from the original object of attention onto a different focal object. Mindfulness, on the other hand, implies regulating the focus and the quality of one’s attention. This implies paying attention to the focal object, but at the same time observing one’s own thoughts and experiences and seeing them as mere mental events. We discuss evidence that distraction and mindfulness modulate the impact of affective information on thoughts, feelings, and behavior. Whereas the two strategies are seemingly opposing in nature, we have found that both distraction and mindfulness can undermine intrusive thinking patterns in response to affective information that normally result in more impulsive behavior. We show how the effectiveness of these strategies is reflected not only in behavioral measures of self-regulation success but in neurophysiological indices as well. Distraction seems to involve the increased engagement of prefrontal brain regions for task-related processing, whereas mindfulness training may affect the connectivity between control and affective brain regions. More broadly speaking, the present chapter shows that combining behavioral and neuroscience measures can be a particularly fruitful approach in understanding how attention strategies impact self-regulation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    For a discussion of the use of the term in Buddhhism, see Lutz, Dunne, & Davidson, 2007.

References

  • Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411, 305–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: Emotion regulation following a focused breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1849–1858.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27–45. doi:10.1177/1073191105283504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bantick, S. J., Wise, R. G., Ploghaus, A., Clare, S., Smith, S. M., & Tracey, I. (2002). Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain, 125, 310–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by Distraction Mind Wandering Facilitates Creative Incubation. Psychological Science, 23, 1117–1122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1281–1289. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C. (2009). ‘Liking’ and ‘wanting’ food rewards: Brain substrates and roles in eating disorders. Physiology & Behavior, 97, 537–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z., Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonanno, G. A., Holen, A., Keltner, D., & Horowitz, M. J. (1995). When avoiding unpleasant emotions might not be such a bad thing—Verbal-autonomic response dissociation and midlife conjugal bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 975–989.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M. (2009). Natural selective attention: Orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology, 46, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(27), 11483–11488. doi:10.1073/pnas.0606552104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. A., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 211–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., Pedersen, W. C., Vasquez, E. A., & Miller, N. (2005). Chewing on it can chew you up: Effects of rumination on triggered displaced aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 969–983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christenfeld, N. (1997). Memory for pain and the delayed effects of distraction. Health Psychology, 16, 327–330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Patel, G.H., & Shulman, G.L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58, 306–324.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: Control, representation, and time course. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 269–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epel, E. S., Puterman, E., Lin, J., Blackburn, E., Lazaro, A., & Mendes, W. B. (2013). Wandering minds and aging cells. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 75–83. doi:10.1177/2167702612460234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erber, R. & Tesser, A. (1992). Task effort and the regulation of mood—The absorption hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 339–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, M., & Eastwood, B. (2005). Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol. Psychopharmacology, 183, 350–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franken, I. H. A. (2003). Drug craving and addiction: Integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 27, 563–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frankenstein, U. N., Richter, W., McIntyre, M. C., & Remy, F. (2001). Distraction modulates anterior cingulate gyrus activations during the cold pressor test. Neuroimage, 14, 827–836.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2011). Attentional states influence early neural responses associated with motivational processes: Local vs. global attentional scope and N1 amplitude to appetitive stimuli. Biological Psychology, 87, 303–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gable P. A., & Harmon-Jones E. (2012). Reducing attentional capture of emotion by broadening attention: Increased global attention reduces early electrophysiological responses to negative stimuli. Biological Psychology, 90, 150–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerin, W., Davidson, K. W., Christenfeld, N. J. S., Goyal, T., & Schwartz, J. E. (2006). The role of angry rumination and distraction in blood pressure recovery from emotional arousal. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 64–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glynn, L. M., Christenfeld, N., & Gerin, W. (2002). The role of rumination in recovery from reactivity: Cardiovascular consequences of emotional states. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 714–726.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, J. A., Courtemanche, J., & Rainville, P. (2011). A nonelaborative mental stance and decoupling of executive and pain-related cortices predicts low pain sensitivity in Zen meditators. Pain, 152, 150–156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunter, R. W., & Bodner, G. E. (2008). How eye movements affect unpleasant memories: Support for a working memory account. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 913–931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasenkamp, W., & Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Effects of meditation experience on functional connectivity of distributed brain networks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Duncan, E., & Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: A fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. NeuroImage, 59(1), 750–760.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., & Dillen, L. V. (2012). Desire The New Hot Spot in Self-Control Research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 317–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, E. A., James, E. L., Coode-Bate, T., & Deeprose, C. (2009). Can playing the computer game Tetris reduce the build-up of flashbacks for trauma? A proposal from cognitive science. PLoS ONE, 4, e4153.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hölzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C., Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T., & Lazar, S. W. (2011). Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 191, 36–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 284–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 109–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, W. A., & Dark, V. J. (1986). Selective attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 37, 43–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Siemer, M. (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation, and dysphoria: Difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 179–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology—Science and Practice, 10, 144–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: Hypoerion Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J., & May, J. (2005). Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychological Review, 112, 446–467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330(6006), 932–932. doi:10.1126/science.1192439.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knudsen, E. I. (2007). Fundamental components of attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 57–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kron, A., Schul, A., Cohen, A., & Hassin, R. R. (2010). Feelings don’t come easy: Studies on the effortful nature of feelings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 520–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D., Carlson, L., Shapiro, S., Carmody, J., Abbey, S., & Devins, G. (2006). The toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 1445–1467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R., Greve, D. N., Treadway, M. T., McGarvey, M., Quinn, B.T., Dusek, J., Benson, H., Rauch, S.L., Moore, C.I., & Fischl, B. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport, 16, 1893.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, C. W., & Cuijpers, P. (2013). A meta-analysis of the contribution of eye movements in processing emotional memories. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 44, 231–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, M. R., Butryn, M. L., Didie, E. R., Annunziato, R. A., Crenand, C. E., Ochner, C. et al. (2009). The power of food scale: A new measure of the psychological influence of the food environment. Appetite, 53, 114–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. D. (2007). Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: An introduction. In The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness. Cambridge: New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, T., & Ward, A. (2004). To eat or not to eat: Implications of the attentional myopia model for restrained eaters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 90–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 519–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness Training Improves Working Memory Capacity and GRE Performance While Reducing Mind Wandering. Psychological Science, 24, 776-781.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally-occurring depressed mood. Cognition & Emotion, 7, 561–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papies, E. K. (2013). Tempting food words activate eating simulations. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 838. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00838.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papies, E. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2014). Grounding Desire and Motivated Behavior: A theoretical framework and empirical evidence. In The Psychology of Desire. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papies, E. K., Stroebe, W., & Aarts, H. (2008). The allure of forbidden food: On the role of attention in self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1283–1292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papies, E. K., Barsalou, L. W., & Custers, R. (2012). Mindful attention prevents mindless impulses. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 291–299. doi:10.1177/1948550611419031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papies, E. K., Pronk, T. M., Keesman, M., & Barsalou, L. W. (2014). The benefits of simply observing: Mindful attention modulates the link between motivation and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, W. C., Gonzales, C., & Miller, N. (2000). The moderating effect of trivial triggering provocation on displaced aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 913–927.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L., & Ungerleider, L.G. (2004). Top-down mechanisms for working memory and attentional processes. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (3rd ed., pp. 919–930). Cambridge, MA, US: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rusting, C. L. & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Regulating responses to anger: Effects of rumination and distraction on angry mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 790–803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siemer, M. (2005). Mood-congruent cognitions constitute mood experience. Emotion, 5, 296–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L. W. (2005). Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1602–1608.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1991). Individual differences in sociosexuality: Evidence for convergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality, 60, 870–883.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheppes, G., & Meiran, N. (2008). There is no such thing as a free lunch: Divergent cognitive costs for online reappraisal and distraction. Emotion, 8, 870–874.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Francis, A. D., Nieuwenhuis, S., Davis, J. M., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biology, 5, e138. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050138.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger, C., Eippert, F., Finsterbusch, J., Bingel, U., Rose, M., & Büchel, C. (2012). Attention modulates spinal cord responses to pain. Current Biology, 22, 1019–1022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D. (1999). Metacognition, mindfulness and the modification of mood disorders. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 6, 146–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615–623. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teper, R., Segal, Z., & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Inside the mindful mind: How mindfulness enhances emotion regulation through improvements in executive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(6), 449–454. doi:10.1177/0963721413495869.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dillen, L. F. & Derks, B. (2012). Working memory load reduces facilitated processing of threatening faces: An ERP study. Emotion, 12, 1340–1349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dillen, L. F., & Koole, S. L. (2007). Clearing the mind: a working memory model of distraction from negative feelings. Emotion, 7, 715–723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dillen, L. F., & Koole, S. L. (2009). How automatic is “automatic vigilance”? The role of working memory in attentional interference of negative information. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1106–1117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dillen, L.F., Heslenfeld, D. J., & Koole, S.L. (2009). Tuning down the emotional brain. NeuroImage, 45, 1212–1219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dillen, L. F., Van der Wal, R., & Van den Bos, K. (2012). On the role of attention and emotion in morality: Attentional control modulates unrelated disgust in moral judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1221–1230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dillen, L. F., Papies, E. K., & Hofmann, W. (2013). Turning a blind eye to temptation: How task load can facilitate self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 427–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dillen, L. F. & Van Steenbergen, H. (2013). Tuning down the hedonic brain. In preparation. Van der Wal, R. C., & van Dillen, L. F. (2013). Leaving a Flat Taste in Your Mouth Task Load Reduces Taste Perception. Psychological Science, 24, 1277–1284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2011). Fixing our focus: Training attention to regulate emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 75–102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, A., & Mann, T. (2000). Don’t mind if I do: Disinhibited eating under cognitive load. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 753–763.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Westbrook, C., Creswell, J. D., Tabibnia, G., Julson, E., Kober, H., & Tindle, H. A. (2013). Mindful attention reduces neural and self-reported cue-induced craving in smokers. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 7384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science, 16, 780–784.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. G. (2008). Mindfulness, depression and modes of mind. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 721–733. doi:10.1007/s10608-008-9204-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lotte F. van Dillen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Conclusion

Conclusion

Integrating insights from psychology and neuroscience , in this chapter, we investigated attention strategies that may facilitate self-regulation, namely distraction, training attention regulation (through mindfulness meditation), and mindful attention . We presented evidence that these strategies affect both the focus and the quality of attention, and as a result, the impact of affective information on thoughts, feelings , and behavior. Whereas seemingly opposing in nature, we have found that both distraction and mindfulness can undermine intrusive thinking patterns in response to affective information that normally result in more impulsive behavior.

We have seen, moreover, that the effectiveness of these strategies is reflected not only in behavioral measures of self-regulation success but in neurophysiological indices as well, and how combining these behavioral and neuroscience measures can help to understand underlying mechanisms of attention strategies. For example, neuroscience studies of both distraction and mindfulness point to the involvement of prefrontal control regions along with brain regions engaged in processing of threat and reward, suggesting that these self-regulation strategies may affect (at least in part) the same neural network. These strategies may, however, engage this network in different ways, as distraction seems to involve the increased engagement of prefrontal brain regions for task-related processing, whereas training attention regulation may affect the connectivity between control and affective brain regions. Preliminary evidence, moreover, points to the possibility that specifically mindful attention affects the reactivity of affective and primary sensory regions, even in the absence of top-down prefrontal control. Examining these attention strategies further, especially their neuropsychological signatures and their long-term effects, may provide new insights in what makes them effective for particular self-regulation situations.

At any moment in time, humans can only keep a few things in their minds. Ironically, this fundamental limitation of human information processing may also have some beneficial consequences. Because processing both affective and non-affective information requires the use of limited attention resources, people can control affective processes via the allocation of attention. By either intentionally directing attention away from affective information or by mindfully observing one’s own reactions to it, the impact of this information on subsequent thought and behavior can be regulated to facilitate long-term goal pursuit and well-being.

Acknowledgment

The preparation of this chapter was partly supported by a grant (VENI-451 − 10-027) from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research to Esther K. Papies.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

van Dillen, L., Papies, E. (2015). From Distraction to Mindfulness: Psychological and Neural Mechanisms of Attention Strategies in Self-Regulation. In: Gendolla, G., Tops, M., Koole, S. (eds) Handbook of Biobehavioral Approaches to Self-Regulation. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-1236-0_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics